Skip to main content
Log in

Was Hercules Happy? Some Answers from a Functional Model of Human Well-being

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The article proposes a functional approach as a framework for the analysis of human well-being. The model posits that the adaptive role of hedonic feelings is to regulate stability and homeostasis in human systems, and that these feelings basically are created in states of equilibrium or assimilation. To regulate change and growth, a distinct set of feelings exists, which may be labeled eudaimonic feelings. Eudaimonic feelings are produced to motivate behavior in challenging environments, when a quick return to equilibrium is dysfunctional, or when accommodation of cognitive structures is needed for a stimulus or event to be perceived as meaningful. It was hypothesized that a trait-like concern for evaluation of outcomes in terms of goodness or badness, referred to as hedonic orientation, will moderate the relation between equilibrium/assimilation and hedonic feelings. The model also includes the concept of eudaimonic orientation, reflecting a stable tendency to get involved in challenging activities and to create and strive after demanding goals. It was hypothesized that a eudaimonic, and not a hedonic, orientation moderates hedonic feelings in challenging episodes. Three different studies gave empirical support to the model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The fable of Hercules exists in many versions (Grayling 2007). The one described here is adapted from Xenophon and retrieved from http://www.rickwalton.com/folktale/holid064.htm.

  2. We are, of course, well aware of arguments against the view of affective bipolarity, such as those presented by Cacioppo et al. (1997) or Schimmack (2008). We nevertheless allay with arguments in favor of the bipolar approach (e.g., Barrett et al. 2007; Kahneman 1999; Russell and Carroll 1999).

  3. Briefly, the folk high schools offer two-semester courses to their students in which they enjoy the freedom to work with topics and questions staff and students feel relevant to study. There is no set curriculum and there are no formal examinations in the system. The schools are open to everyone. Usually the minimum age for admission is 17 years.

References

  • Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The work preference inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 950–967. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinwall, L. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (Eds.). (2003). A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology. Washington: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338–375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe-Center, J. G. (1965). The psychology og pleasantness and unpleasantness. New York: Russell & Russell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. London: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E., & Madson, K. B. (Eds.). (1973). Pleasure, reward, preference: Their nature, determinants, and role in behavior. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52, 106–128. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00014-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boldero, J., & Francis, J. (2002). Goals, standards, and the self: Reference values serving different functions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 232–241. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In I. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaption level theory. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabanac, M. (1992). Pleasure. The common currency. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 155, 173–200. doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80594-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualization and measure: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, K. S., Dutton, J. E., & Quinn, R. E. (Eds.). (2003). Positive organizational scholarship. Foundations of a new dicipline. San Francisco: Berret-Koehler Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Avon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 1–11. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. doi:10.1007/BF01207052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229–259. doi:10.1023/A:1019672513984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money. Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 1–31. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. The new psychology of success. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckblad, G. (1963). The attractiveness of uncertainty. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 4, 1–13. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.1963.tb01300.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckblad, G. (1981). Scheme theory. A conceptual framework for cognitive-motivational processes. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1994). All emotions are basic. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion. Fundamental questions (pp. 15–19). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filseth, G. (2001, September 9). Flyktninger til dødsdømt øy [Refugees to a doomed island]. Aftenposten, p. 18.

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2000). Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-being. Prevention & Treatment, 3, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion. Fundamental questions (pp. 112–122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaver, W. W., & Mandler, G. (1987). Play it again Sam: On liking music. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 259–282. doi:10.1080/02699938708408051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H., & Opper, S. (1969). Piaget’s theory of intellectual development. An introduction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giovanola, B. (2005). Personhood and human richness: Good and well-being in the capability approach and beoynd. Review of Social Economy, LXIII, 249–267. doi:10.1080/00346760500130416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grayling, A. C. (2007). The choice of Hercules. Pleasure, duty and the good life in the 21st century. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haybron, D. M. (2000). Two philosophical problems in the study of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1, 207–225. doi:10.1023/A:1010075527517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. (2008). Life goals matter to happiness: A revision of set-point theory. Social Indicators Research, 86, 213–232. doi:10.1007/s11205-007-9138-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 79–109. doi:10.1006/cogp.1998.0708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological Review, 113, 439–460. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • IPIP. (2002). International personality item pool. A scientific collaboration for the development of advanced measurement of personality traits and other individual differences. Available from http://ipip.ori.org/.

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, V. S. (2003). The origin and function of pleasure. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 169–179. doi:10.1080/02699930302290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20, 3–24. doi:10.1257/089533006776526030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 305, 1776–1780. doi:10.1126/science.1103572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Biswas-Diener, R., & King, L. A. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: The costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 219–233. doi:10.1080/17439760802303044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P., & Fincham, F. D. (2004). Curiosity and exploration: Facilitating positive subjective experience and personal growth opportunities. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82, 291–305. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8203_05.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2007). Curiosity and pathways to well-being and meaning in life: Traits, states, and everyday behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 159–173. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9068-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, A., & Kenny, C. (2006). Life, liberty, and the pursuit of utility. Happiness in philosophical and economic thought. Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, C. L. M. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: A complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist, 62, 95–108. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubovy, M. (1999). On the pleasures of the mind. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 135–154). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. E. (1996). Quality of life and quality of persons: A new role for government? In A. Offer (Ed.), In pursuit of the quality of life (pp. 256–293). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129–141. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1103_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lischetzke, T., & Eid, M. (2006). Why extraverts are happier than introverts: The role of mood regulation. Journal of Personality, 74, 1127–1161. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00405.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2007). Long-term disability is associated with lasting changes in subjective well-being: Evidence from two nationally representative longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 717–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Clark, A. E. (2006). Do people really adapt to marriage? Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 405–426. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9001-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803–855. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison of happy and unhappy individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 988–1007. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manchin, R. (2007, October). More perspectives on global well-being: Reporting on well-being in European cities: What exactly do we measure? Paper presented at the Sixth International Positive Psychology Summit, Washington, DC.

  • Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body. Psychology of emotion and stress. London: W.W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, G. (1998). Pleasure. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy (pp. 448–451). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, C. N., & Gowdy, J. M. (2000). Paradise for sale. A parable of nature. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science, 6, 10–19. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00298.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nozick, R. (1974). Anarchy, state, and utopia. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, M. C. (2007). Mill between Aristotle and Bentham. In L. Bruni & P. L. Porta (Eds.), Economics and happiness. Framing the analysis (pp. 170–183). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes. The psychology of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., Pak, H.-J., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, Ill: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and judgement. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R. (1992). A structuralist reconstruction of Wundt’s three-dimensional theory of emotion. In H. Westmeyer (Ed.), The structuralist program in psychology. Foundations and applications (pp. 141–189). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (2005). Emotions explained. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (1999). Preadaptation and the puzzles and properties of pleasure. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 109–133). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. doi:10.1037/h0077714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. D. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Living well: A self-determination theory perspective in eudaimonia. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 139–170. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9023-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C., & Singer, B. H. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: A eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 13–39. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9019-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2008). The structure of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 97–123). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2008). The influence of environment and personality on the affective and cognitive component of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 89, 41–60. doi:10.1007/s11205-007-9230-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlosberg, H. (1954). Three dimensions of emotion. Psychological Review, 61, 81–88. doi:10.1037/h0054570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze, G. (1995). Motivation. Homeostatic mechanisms may instigate and shape adaptive behaviors through the generation of hedonic states. In R. Wong (Ed.), Biological perspectives on motivated activities (pp. 265–288). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness. Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for deep fulfilment. London: Nicholas Brealey Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2001). Interest and interests: The psychology of constructive capriciousness. Review of General Psychology, 5, 270–290. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.3.270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89–102. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sumner, L. W. (1996). Welfare, happiness, and ethics. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tatarkiewich, W. (1976). Analysis of happiness. Nijhoff: den Haag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A., & Martin, L. (1996). The psychology of evaluation. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology. Handbook of basic principles (pp. 400–431). London: The Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thyer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. S. (1970). Affect as the primary motivational system. In M. B. Arnold (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 101–110). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, S. A., Jr, & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must interesting things be pleasant? A test of competing appraisal structures. Emotion, 6, 670–674. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Lavrakas, P. J. (2000). Survey research. In H. T. Reis & C. J. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 223–252). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J. (2004). Subjective well-being versus self-actualization: Using the flow-simplex to promote a conceptual clarification of subjective quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 65, 299–331. doi:10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003910.26194.ef.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J., Öhlman, H. I., & Wang, A. L. (2009). Life satisfaction is not a balanced estimator of the good life: Evidence from reaction time measures and self-reported emotions. Journal of Happiness Studies. doi:10.1007/s10902-007-9058-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J., & Søholt, Y. (2008). Life satisfaction goes with pleasure and personal growth goes with interest. Further arguments for the usefulness of distinguishing eudaimonic well-being from hedonic well-being (submitted).

  • Vittersø, J., & Straume, L. V. (2009). Towards a measurement tool for eudaimonic well-being: The personal growth composite (manuscript in preparation).

  • Vittersø, J., Vorkinn, M., Vistad, O. I., & Vaagland, J. (2000). Tourist experiences and attractions. Annals of Tourism Research, 27, 432–450. doi:10.1016/S0160-7383(99)00087-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. D. (1981). The quest for the inverted U. In H. I. Day (Ed.), Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (pp. 39–70). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (Eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678–691. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: A eudaimonist’s perspective. Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 234–252. doi:10.1080/17439760802303002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. (2002). Positive affectivity: The disposition to experience pleasurable emotional states. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 106–119). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weitz, J. (1952). A neglected concept in the study of job satisfaction. Personnel Psychology, 5, 201–205. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1952.tb01012.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 989–1000. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgement. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation. Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1896). Outline of psychology. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, P. T. (1961). Motivation and emotion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 2–27. doi:10.1037/h0025848. Monograph Supplement.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joar Vittersø.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Vittersø, J., Søholt, Y., Hetland, A. et al. Was Hercules Happy? Some Answers from a Functional Model of Human Well-being. Soc Indic Res 95, 1–18 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9447-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9447-4

Keywords

Navigation